Friday, 23 May 2014

One-minute animations on theories of religion

Today I came across these adorable but also informative one-minute overviews of (evolutionary) theories of religon. Though they are by no means anywhere close to exhaustive, they're fun to watch, and give you a quick idea on some of the major perspectives on the evolution of religion! Enjoy!

Religion as social control
Karl Marx's idea of 'religion is opium of the people', how it oppresses the workers as well as making them feel better about being oppressed.



Religion as ritual
Sociologist (Isidore) Auguste (Marie François Xavier) Comte's idea of how religion develops along with science, and that ritual becomes an important part of society in the process.



Religion as a mother
Swiss antiquarian and Roman law professor J. J. Bachofer’s theories of matriarchy.


Religion as a virus
Atheist and evolutionary biologist Richard Dawkin's idea of religion as a virus, spread through 'memes'.



0 comments

Monday, 10 February 2014

Nijmegen Lectures - Day 3: The evolution of culture without miracles

Part of the Nijmegen Lectures 2014, given by Professor Russell Gray. See previous post

Bio-cultural analogy

On the third and final day of the Nijmegen Lectures, Russell Gray expanded on the evolution of culture ‘without miracles’. There has been a debate for some time whether cultural evolution can be likened to biological or genetic evolution. Darwin himself had this to say about it:

"The formation of different languages and of distinct species, and the proofs that both have been developed through a gradual process, are curiously parallel." (Darwin, 1871)                                         
 A few of the major arguments against the analogy are:
  • Variation in cultural ideas is not as random (as genetic mutations), but rather intentionally designed
  • Cultural features have low fidelity transmission, including:
    • Information loss
    • Vertical AND horizontal transmission
    • Blending (copying one ancestor, like genes, but also copying many ancestors, unlike genes)
People from a tribe in Northern Thailand (August 2008)

Transmission fidelity

Russell discussed these points briefly in his lecture. He brought forward some ideas from Dan Sperber's 'Explaining Culture' (book) to show that perhaps we should not see cultural variance as being replicated or copied, but rather as interpreted, or reconstructed. When you reconstruct, you can take a lot of variants you come across, and then make your own out of that set (blending) - this suggests a difference from high fidelity copying. Similarly, when we tell stories (a cultural feature), we may not even always tell the same story ourselves. Still, Russell argues, such cultural attractors create discrete units of culture that are stable despite noisy transmission.

One way such units of culture may stabilize could be on the population level: Henrich and Boyd (2002) have argued that accurate replication need not occur at the level of cultural innovations, but rather that conformist bias can stabilize these units at the population level. Prestige bias is another form of such stabilization, whereby the most successful cultural features are blended, resulting in adaptive evolution. In this way transmission fidelity can be very low at the individual level but high at the population level.

Horizontal transmission / hybridization

Another issue is that cultural trees don't just diverge, but syncretize and form anastomoses too (when two branches of a tree come back together again).  In this sense, the tree of culture is a ramification of assimilations and acculturations (Kroeber, 1948). This led Stephen Jay Gould (1987) to oppose the bio-cult evolution analogy; he argued that biological lineages become separate, and don't amalgamate (except through rare hybridization), while cultural evolution has rampant transmission and anastomosis. Russell argued that this judgement was made too fast: while Gould worked on vertebrates, the evolutionary trees of virus are a lot 'messier' and can include horizontal transmission as well (and may be more similar to cultural evolution). 

But most of all we need methods to test these different ideas! We need to develop data and methods to test whether there is cross-lineage transfer, what the fidelity of cultural features is, etc. 

It seems that some cultural aspects show high fidelity transmission - there are certain words in languages in the Pacific that after 446,000 years of cultural evolution are still very similar. That is striking. It may be that some aspects are transmitted with high fidelity (traditional decorations, for example) and others via horizontal transmission (iPods, for example). How can we look at this? Russell suggested that rather than looking at these aspects by means of evolutionary trees or 'waves', perhaps we should use networks (to capture the combination of horizontal and vertical transmission). One way to do this is via NeighbourNet, developed by Bryant and Moulton (2002). 

Network analyses

This method was developed for biological data, to understand bacterial and viral lineage conflicts. It allows one to visualize whether there is strong historical lineage or more conflicting signal. When there is a good match, the data looks tree-like, but when there are conflicting signals, the figure looks more like a network, or a spider web. In this way, hybrid origins of languages can for example also be visualized, which shows up as double lines grouping a language with two sets of languages (parallel lines represent spliuts, and the line length represents the split weight) - see Gray, Greenhill and Bryant (2010). Such a method may prove very useful for the science of cultural evolution, and even the story of Little Red Riding Hood has been analysed with it so far (Tehrani, 2013 - open access article).

So, to round off the final talk of the Nijmegen Lectures, there is some evidence that cultural evolution may be likened to biological evolution - in some case more like the biological evolution of bacterias and viruses. In either case, we need methods to test such ideas directly, and network analyses may be helpful to this regard.

I hope you have enjoyed reading about the Nijmegen Lectures, I certainly enjoyed being there! /V.

P.S. For those readers who have noticed the tags used for this post - what's the difference between 'evolution of culture' and 'cultural evolution'? Merely in terms of blog tags, I am trying to distinguish between discussing (specific) examples of cultural features and their evolution ('evolution of culture') and the discussion of cultural vs biological evolution ('cultural evolution'). Cheers!

Selected references

- Boyd R., Borgerhoff Mulder M., Durham W. H., Richerson P. J. 1997. Are cultural phylogenies possible? In Human by nature, between biology and the social sciences (eds Weingart P., Richerson P. J., Mitchell S. D., Maasen S., editors. ), pp. 355–386, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- Bryant, D., & Moulton, V. (2002). NeighborNet: An agglomerative method for the construction of planar phylogenetic networks. In Algorithms in Bioinformatics (pp. 375-391). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man. D. Appleton and Company.
Gray, R.D., Greenhill, S.J., and Bryant, D. (2010). On the shape and fabric of human history. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London, B, 365, 3923-3933.
Henrich, J. & R. Boyd (2002) On Modeling Cognition and Culture: Why replicators are not necessary for cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture 2(2): 87-112. 
Pagel, M., & Mace, R. (2004). The cultural wealth of nations. Nature, 428(6980), 275-278.
- Sperber, Dan. (1996) Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Blackwell Publishers.
Tehrani, J. J. (2013). The Phylogeny of Little Red Riding Hood. PloS One, 8(11), e78871.
0 comments

Thursday, 6 February 2014

Darwinizing culture (video)

On the third day of the Nijmegen Lectures, Russell Gray talked about the evolution of culture (including language). A highly informative video that covers some similar material, can be found through UCLA (Youtube), posted below. If you are interested on how phylogenetic methods can be applied to cultural features, I recommend you watch this video. Enjoy!



For the discussion afterwards, go here.


0 comments

Nijmegen Lectures - Day 2: The evolution of language without miracles

Part of the Nijmegen Lectures 2014, given by Professor Russell Gray. See previous post

On the second day of the Nijmegen Lectures, Russell Gray talked about the evolution of language ‘without miracles’. He started with ubiquitous tension in language research between unity and variety, between similarity and diversity. While languages have striking similarities, the diversity of languages is also impressive: it is typically estimating that there are over 7,000 languages in the world, with some areas of the world having a particularly dense variety (i.e., 500 languages in Vanuatu alone).

image source
Interestingly, in linguistics, people have typically tried to focus on the similarities: while Chomsky postulated a ‘universal grammar’ applying to all langauges, Greenberg tried to find probabilistic constraints. The idea behind these set-ups was that, despite the seemingly large variety at the surface, underlying on these so-called 'language phenotypes' are very similar 'language genotypes', constrained by the innate structure of the mind.

In (evolutionary) biology on the other hand, people have generally emphasized the variety of species rather than similarities between them. Indeed, since Darwin, the variation is considered to be the central reality, not an irrelevant disturbance to take away from a sort of underlying Platonic form (Lewinton, 1983). According to Darwin, if there is any unity of type, this is to be explained by unity of descent.

Alright, but what does this tell us about the evolution of language? Well if there is one principle underlying all languages, then one mutation could perhaps suffice to give language to all humans (as is argued, if somewhat simplified, by Chomsky). If, on the other hand, you suggest that languages evolved in incremental steps, this provides more leeway to arrive at the languages seen today.

However, one of the problems with comparing languages and tracking their evolution is that languages are not independent. This is typically referred to as Galton’s problem: Cultures are not statistically independent, their features are not independent data points. Thus, the history of the languages matters, and sampling from a wide variety of language families doesn’t solve the problem (because even apparently shared dependencies can have different historical pathways and vice versa). 

In order to get around this issue, Dunn, Greenhill, Levinson and Gray have applied phylogenetic methods (from evolutionary biology) to word-order in languages around the world. By fitting trees probabilistically on phylogenies, they were able to test whether instances of language evolution were correlated or uncorrelated. Through this research, they show that functional dependencies between word-order traits are lineage-specific (i.e., to what 'family' the language belongs) rather than universal tendencies, as argued by Greenberg. This means that word-order is determined through cultural evolution rather than being driven by innate constraints of the mind. 

Another example of how cultural evolution may drive language evolution is the diversification of Austroasiatic languages (Gray, Drummond and Greenhill, 2009). Russell and colleagues used Bayesian phylogenetic methods to construct a phylogeny of 400 languages on the basis of basic vocabulary. Basic vocabulary is used because it typically evolves slowly and is less likely to be borrowed. Then, given their priors and the model, they tested whether which of two prior hypotheses (the 'pulse-pause' model or the 'slow-boat' model, see paper) was most likely to be true. They found that Austroasiatic languges change through time, though through a constant diversification, but rather by an initial burst of diversification which then plateaus out. Then, after this 'pause', there is another increase in diversification, etc. This model fits best with the 'pulse-pause' model, which was also correct about the origin (Taiwan) and trajectory (via Philippines, Indonesia, and so on) of this language diversification. This rises the question however, what is driving the increases of diversification? One hypothesis is that it may follow the development of rice cultivation: When rice cultivation becomes more serieous and systematic, people are tied to a place, and the population can increase, which leads to a density increase, and possibly an increase in diversification of language. 

In sum, I believe that Russell and colleagues have convincingly shown that phylogenetic methods can be applied to cultural phenomena as language. Though reported somewhat simplified here, I encourage people to read Gray, Drummond and Greenhill (2009) and Gray & Atkinson (2003) to learn more about these exciting studies (and to learn more about validation techniques and robustness checks they employed).

For future research of the evolution of language then, we need to figure out how language can come about from a continuous system, through incremental steps in the brain. Thus, we need a better linguistic theory so that we know what to look for in the brain. Important is that we start appreciate the wide variety of languages in the world again, rather than reducing all of these to a variation on, randomly, English.

Selected references

- Chomsky, N. (2010). Some simple evo devo theses: how true might they be for language? The evolution of human language (eds. Larson R. K., Deprez V., Yamakido H., editors. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press), 45-62.
- Dunn, M., Greenhill, S.J., Levinson, S.C. and Gray, R.D. (2011). Evolved structure of language shows lineage-specific trends in word-order universals. Nature, 473, 79–82. doi:10.1038/nature09923
- Gray, R. D., Drummond, A. J., & Greenhill, S. J. (2009). Language phylogenies reveal expansion pulses and pauses in Pacific settlement. Science323(5913), 479-483.
- Levinson, S.C. and Gray, R.D. (2012). Tools from evolutionary biology shed new light on the diversification of languages. Trends in Cognitive Sciences16 (3), 167-173 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.007

Further reading
- Gray, R. D., & Atkinson, Q. D. (2003). Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo-European origin. Nature426(6965), 435-439.
0 comments

Monday, 27 January 2014

Nijmegen Lectures - Day 1: The evolution of cognition without miracles

Part of the Nijmegen Lectures 2014, given by Professor Russell Gray. See previous post

Day 1 of the Nijmegen Lectures covered lots of interesting work by Russell and his colleagues, in particular Dr. Alex Taylor, on corvids - they've shown a whole range of incredible things that the New Caledonian crow can do. But first, Russell gave an overview of one of the major current debates in animal cognition: Do animals have human cognition, or a significantly reduced version of it? 
Source: gardenofeaden.blogspot.com
Russell described the two major groups in this debate: the 'romantics', who believe that animals are capable of human cognition, and the 'killjoys', who suggest that seemingly intelligent behaviour can be explained by means of simpler forms of cognition, such as associative learning. Before expanding on this debate, Russell forecasted that he would argue that these views are both fundamentally unsatisfactory, and that rather, we should look at incremental improvements, an intermediate form of cognition - i.e., no miracles in evolution. We'll return to that later.

The romantic view is that humans are fundamentally similar to species. For every characteristic that was once claimed to be uniquely human, people have claimed they have found animal counterparts. Take for example tool use. While first denounced uniquely human by Louis Leakey (who coined the phrase 'Man the Tool Maker'), Jane Goodall quickly debunked this by showing that chimpanzees use tools. (Interestingly, Louis Leakey responded to this by stating: "Now we must redefine tool, redefine Man, or accept chimpanzees as humans.") But people have argued similar cases for theory of mind, causative cognition, even recursion.

The killjoys on the other hand claim that simpler mechanisms than full-blown human cognition can explain animal behaviour observed in experiments (typically associative learning). As earlier as 1894, it was argued that: "In no case may we interpret an action as the outcome of the exercise of one which stands lower in the psychological scale." (Morgan, 1894, 53). This statement is referred to as 'Morgan's Canon'.

Through discussing a series of very interesting experiments (see references), Russell showed that he had been on both sides of the debate. However, he also showed that both camps had problems they need to deal with: The romantic view for example is evolutionary implausible - evolution does not proceed in miraculous leaps. Furthermore, claims of human-like "super cognitive" abilities such as 'insight' don't identify cognitive mechanisms (that can be tested). The 'killjoy' view on the other hand is not evolutionary plausible either - assuming that many animals have identical cognitive abilities is not plausible (for one, they are physically different, too). The biggest failure of this view is that it leaves differences between species unexplained.

Rather, Russell suggests to follow the following research program, 'without miracles' but with incremental steps: For example, New Caledonian crows may be where they are now through small (morphological) tweaks (such as the change of the beak of the New Caledonian crow, and its more binocular view), small changes in the brain (Medina et al., 2013), and tweaks in the developmental course (Kenward et al., 2011). This suggests that the animal cognition question raised above is just the wrong question.

In future research, then, we should move from so-called 'gee whizz' experiments to signature testing. That is, develop a theory-driven battery of tests, that preferably can be administered to a range of different species. While administring these tests, both the success and also the failures should be monitored. Finally, more comparative (phylogenetic) research is required, including genomics, neuroscience and developmental research. In sum, abandon the dichonomous debate on romantics vs killjoys, but take a more practical stance where verifiable hypotheses are brought forward and tested. Rather than comparing humans to other species, we should do signature testing and develop a good understanding of the biology of related species to the one we are testing, to discover the incremental, intermediate steps which may have taken place.


Selected references

- Holzhaider, J. C., Hunt, G. R., Campbell, V. M., & Gray, R. D. (2008). Do wild New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides) attend to the functional properties of their tools?. Animal Cognition, 11(2), 243-254.

- Hunt, G.R., Gray R.D. (2006). Tool manufacture by New Caledonian crows: chipping away at
human uniqueness. Acta Zoologica Sinica 52 (Suppl.): 622-625.
- Kenward, B., Schloegl, C., Rutz, C., Weir, A. A., Bugnyar, T., & Kacelnik, A. (2011). On the evolutionary and ontogenetic origins of tool‐oriented behaviour in New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 102(4), 870-877.
- Medina, F. S., Hunt, G. R., Gray, R. D., Wild, J. M., & Kubke, M. F. (2013). Perineuronal satellite neuroglia in the telencephalon of New Caledonian crows and other Passeriformes: evidence of satellite glial cells in the central nervous system of healthy birds?. PeerJ, 1, e110.
- Taylor, A. H., Knaebe, B., & Gray, R. D. (2012). An end to insight? New Caledonian crows can spontaneously solve problems without planning their actions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1749), 4977-4981.
- Taylor, A.H., Miller, R. and Gray, R.D. (2012). New Caledonian crows reason about hidden causal agents. PNAS published ahead of print September 17, 2012, doi:10.1073/pnas.1208724109

Further reading

- Heyes, C. (2012). Simple minds: a qualified defence of associative learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1603), 2695-2703.

0 comments

Nijmegen Lectures 2014: Russell Gray on Evolution

Photo from auckland.ac.nz
Today I went to the first day of the Nijmegen Lectures 2014. This year's Nijmegen Lectures (organised by the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics) are given by Professor Russel Gray, from the School of Psychology at the University of Auckland in New Zealand, and are titled 'No miracles! A Darwinian view of the evolution of cognition, language and culture'.

Russell defined himself as an evolutionary biologist and psychologist in the lectures, which is probably modest. His work has contributed to the fields of linguistics, animal cognition, philosophy of biology and behavioural phylogenetics. He is best known for his work with corvids (the New Caledonian crow) and the application of computational evolutionary methods to language.

I was really happy to hear about these lectures as they are close to my hometown where I am currently residing, and because I always like to hear talks by Russell and his colleagues. While I was doing my PhD in Auckland, I followed the postgraduate course Evolutionary Psychology which that year was given by Russell and Quentin Atkinson - before the semester was over I knew that this is what I wanted to do. I finished my PhD first though of course (and attended some more of Russell and Quentin's lab meetings), and am now looking for a postdoc in evolutionary psychology/anthropology. It's the best!  

Time to write down the report of the first day. 
0 comments

Wednesday, 22 January 2014

Welcome to HBH!


Hi and welcome to Here Be Humans!

It's January 2014, and I've decided to start this blog to write about previous and current developments in research on humans and human evolution that I've come across.

This is a rather broad description, but will covers my interests in anthropology, linguistics, evolutionary research, psychology and cognitive neuroscience, and favourite topics like religion, creativity, the arts, language, literature, history, cognition, philosophy, etc.

I intend to include commentaries on scholarly articles, book reviews, conference/talks reviews, etcetera. Please note that all interpretations (including potentially erroneous ones!) are mine, something which may be particularly important for talk reports.

I hope you enjoy the reading, and please leave a comment if you want to join the discussion!

Valerie

Lotus in a pond in Vietnam (December 2013)


0 comments
 

Copyright 2008 All Rights Reserved Blogger Template by Bloganol dot com